DE eng

Search in the Catalogues and Directories

Page: 1 2 3 4 5...10
Hits 1 – 20 of 183

1
Correction to: How sensitive are adults to the role of morphology in spelling? [<Journal>]
Treiman, Rebecca [Verfasser]; Wolter, Sloane [Verfasser]; Kessler, Brett [Verfasser]
DNB Subject Category Language
Show details
2
Letter names in children's spelling ...
Treiman, Rebecca. - : Open Science Framework, 2021
BASE
Show details
3
How sensitive are adults to the role of morphology in spelling? [<Journal>]
Treiman, Rebecca [Verfasser]; Wolter, Sloane [Verfasser]; Kessler, Brett [Verfasser]
DNB Subject Category Language
Show details
4
Word class and spelling in English ...
Treiman, Rebecca; Jewell, Rebecca; Berg, Kristian. - : Open Science Framework, 2020
BASE
Show details
5
Cues to stress in English spelling ...
Treiman, Rebecca; Kessler, Brett. - : Open Science Framework, 2020
BASE
Show details
6
Letter Teaching in Parent–Child Conversations
In: Early Child Res Q (2020)
BASE
Show details
7
Letter Features as Predictors of Letter-Name Acquisition in Four Languages with Three Scripts
In: Sci Stud Read (2020)
BASE
Show details
8
Use of Letter Names Benefits Young Children’s Spelling ...
Treiman, Rebecca; Wolter, Sloane. - : SAGE Journals, 2019
BASE
Show details
9
Use of Letter Names Benefits Young Children’s Spelling ...
Treiman, Rebecca; Wolter, Sloane. - : SAGE Journals, 2019
BASE
Show details
10
Children benefit from morphological relatedness independently of orthographic relatedness when they learn to spell new words
In: ISSN: 0022-0965 ; EISSN: 1096-0457 ; Journal of Experimental Child Psychology ; https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-03244841 ; Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, Elsevier, 2018, 171, pp.71-83. &#x27E8;10.1016/j.jecp.2018.02.003&#x27E9; (2018)
Abstract: We examined whether French children in Grades 3 and 5 (aged ∼ 8–11 years) benefit from morphological relatedness beyond orthographic relatedness in the implicit learning of new spellings. Children silently read stories that included two target nonwords. One nonword was in an opaque condition in that nothing in the story could justify the spelling of its final sound. The other nonword was in either a morphological condition (for children in the morphological group) or an orthographic condition (for children in the orthographic group). In the morphological condition, the final spelling of the target nonword was justified by two morphologically related nonwords. For example, coirardage, obtained by adding the suffix age to coirard, designates the coirard’s song and justifies the final silent d of coirard. The orthographic condition included two nonwords that were orthographically but not morphologically related to the target. For example, the coirard’s song was coirardume, obtained by adding ume, which is not a suffix, to coirard. Then, 30 min after reading the stories, children were asked to choose the correct spelling of each nonword from among three phonologically plausible alternatives (e.g., coirard, coirars, coirar). In the morphological group, both third and fifth graders more often selected the correct spellings for items presented in the morphological condition than for items presented in the opaque condition. In the orthographic group, the results were very similar in the opaque and orthographic conditions. The findings show that the benefit of morphological relatedness in the implicit learning of new spellings cannot be reduced to orthographic relatedness.
Keyword: [SHS.PSY]Humanities and Social Sciences/Psychology; Implicit learning; Morphology; Orthographic learning; Self-teaching; Spelling; Spelling acquisition
URL: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2018.02.003
https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-03244841
BASE
Hide details
11
Children benefit from morphological relatedness independently of orthographic relatedness when they learn to spell new words
In: ISSN: 0022-0965 ; EISSN: 1096-0457 ; Journal of Experimental Child Psychology ; https://halshs.archives-ouvertes.fr/halshs-01851979 ; Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, Elsevier, 2018 (2018)
BASE
Show details
12
Response times in CVC naming study, Treiman et al. (1995) ...
BASE
Show details
13
Response times in CVC naming study, Treiman et al. (1995) ...
BASE
Show details
14
Statistical learning and spelling: Evidence from Brazilian prephonological spellers
BASE
Show details
15
Linguistics and reading
In: The handbook of linguistics (Oxford, 2017), p. 617-626
MPI für Psycholinguistik
Show details
16
Statistical Learning and Spelling: Older Prephonological Spellers Produce More Wordlike Spellings than Younger Prephonological Spellers
BASE
Show details
17
Measures of Kindergarten Spelling and Their Relations to Later Spelling Performance
BASE
Show details
18
Young Children’s Knowledge of the Symbolic Nature of Writing
BASE
Show details
19
The Oxford handbook of reading
Pollatsek, Alexander; Treiman, Rebecca. - New York : Oxford University Press, 2015
MPI für Psycholinguistik
Show details
20
The Oxford handbook of reading
Treiman, Rebecca (Hrsg.); Pollatsek, Alexander (Hrsg.). - New York [u.a.] : Oxford Univ. Press, 2015
BLLDB
UB Frankfurt Linguistik
Show details

Page: 1 2 3 4 5...10

Catalogues
5
0
46
0
3
0
0
Bibliographies
99
0
0
0
0
0
0
0
15
Linked Open Data catalogues
0
Online resources
0
0
0
0
Open access documents
33
0
0
0
0
© 2013 - 2024 Lin|gu|is|tik | Imprint | Privacy Policy | Datenschutzeinstellungen ändern